Beja, Edsel Jr. (2013): Focusing on the Focusing Illusion...
Preview |
PDF
MPRA_paper_44366.pdf Download (169kB) | Preview |
Abstract
The paper presents evidence that validate the focusing illusion. Specifically, the forecasted impact of a basketball championship on students’ subjective well-being was exaggerated because of their intense focus on the event. However, the self-reported states of being for life domains not closely associated with the focal event remained stable throughout the survey periods. Further analysis of the data finds that the exaggerated level of subjective well-being brought about by the focusing illusion had minimal spillover effects on the reported levels of subjective well-being for the other life domains.
Item Type: | MPRA Paper |
---|---|
Original Title: | Focusing on the Focusing Illusion... |
Language: | English |
Keywords: | Focusing illusion; spillover; subjective well-being; happiness; life domains |
Subjects: | C - Mathematical and Quantitative Methods > C8 - Data Collection and Data Estimation Methodology ; Computer Programs C - Mathematical and Quantitative Methods > C9 - Design of Experiments > C90 - General D - Microeconomics > D0 - General > D03 - Behavioral Microeconomics: Underlying Principles D - Microeconomics > D1 - Household Behavior and Family Economics > D10 - General D - Microeconomics > D6 - Welfare Economics I - Health, Education, and Welfare > I0 - General Y - Miscellaneous Categories > Y8 - Related Disciplines |
Item ID: | 44366 |
Depositing User: | Edsel Beja, Jr. |
Date Deposited: | 14 Feb 2013 09:45 |
Last Modified: | 01 Oct 2019 08:51 |
References: | Buehler, R. and McFarland, C. (2001). Intensity bias in affective forecasting: The role of temporal focus, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(11): 1480-1493 Campbell, D. (1976). Assessing the impact of planned social change, Occasional Paper No. 8, Social Research and Public Policies, Hanover, NH: Dartmouth College Costa, P. and McCrae, R. (1988). Personality in adulthood: A six-year longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO personality inventory, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4): 853-863 Diener, E. and Emmons, R. (1985). The independence of positive and negative affect, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(5): 71-75 Diener, E. and Larsen, R. (1984). Temporal stability and cross-situational consistency of affective, behavioral, and cognitive responses, Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 47(4): 580-592 Dolan, P. and Metcalfe, R. (2010). ‘Oops. . .I did it again’: Repeated focusing effects in reports of happiness, Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(4): 732-737 Dolan, P. and Powdthavee, N. (2011). Thinking about It: A note on attention and well-being losses from unemployment, Applied Economics Letters, 19(4): 325-328 Eastwick, P., Finkel, E., Krishnamurti, T., and Loewenstein, G. (2008). Mispredicting distress following romantic breakup: Revealing the time course of the affective forecasting error, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3): 800-807 Gilbert, D. and Wilson, T (2000). Miswanting: Some problems in the forecasting of future affective states, in J. Forgas (ed.), Thinking and feeling: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 178-97), Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Gilbert, D., Pinel, E., Wilson, T., and Blumberg, S. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3): 617-638 Goodhart, C. (1975). Monetary relationships: A new form of Threadneedle Street, Papers in Monetary Economics No. 1, Sydney: Reserve Bank of Australia Hooghe, M. (2012). I am happy, hope you’re happy too: Examining the different dynamics of individual subjective well-being and view on society, Journal of Happiness Studies, 13(1): 17-29 Kahneman, D., Krueger, A., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., and Stone, A. (2006). Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion, Science, 312(5782): 1908-1910 Kermer, D., Driver-Linn, E., Wilson, T., and Gilbert, D. (2006). Loss aversion is an affective forecasting error, Psychological Sciences, 17(8): 649-653 Loewenstein, G., O’Donoghue, T., and Rabin, M. (2003). Projection bias in predicting future utility, Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118(4): 1209-248 Lucas, R. (1976). Econometric policy evaluation: a critique, in K. Brunner and A. Meltzer (ed.), The Phillips Curve and Labor Markets (pp. 19–46), New York, NY: North Holland. Lucas, R., Diener, E., and Suh, E. (1996). “Discriminant validity of well-being measures,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(3): 616-628 Michalos, A. (1985). Multiple discrepancies theory, Social Indicators Research, 16(4): 347-413 Nakazato, N., Schimmack, U., and Oishi, S. (2011). Effect of changes in living conditions on well-being: A prospective top-down bottom-up model, Social Indicators Research, 100(1): 115-135 Riis, J., Loewenstein, G., Baron, J., Jepson, C., Fajerlin, A., and Ubel, P. (2005). Ignorance of hedonic adaptation to hemodialysis: A study using ecological momentary assessment, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(1): 3-9 Schimmack, U., Diener, E., and Oishi, S. (2002). Life satisfaction is a momentary judgment and a stable personality characteristic: The use of chronically accessible and stable sources, Journal of Personality, 70(3): 345-384 Schkade, D. and Kahneman, D. (1998). Does living in California make people happy? A focusing illusion in judgments of life satisfaction, Psychological Sciences, 9(5):340-346 Schwartz, N. and Clore, G. (1983). Mood attribution, judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3): 513-523 Schwarz, N., Strack, F., Commer, D., and Wagner, D. (1987). Soccer, rooms, and the quality of your life: Mood effects on judgments of satisfaction with life in general and with specific domains, European Journal of Social Psychology, 17(1): 69-79 Smith, D., Sherriff, R., Damschroder, L., Loewenstein, G., and Ubel, P. (2006). Misremembering colostomies? Former patients give lower utility ratings than do current patients, Health Psychology, 25(6): 688-695 Strack, F., Martin, L., and Schwarz, N. (1988). Priming and communication: Social determinants of information use in judgments of life satisfaction, European Journal of Social Psychology, 18(5): 429-442 Wilson, T. and Gilbert, D. (2003). Affective forecasting, in M. Zanna (ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 345-411), San Diego: Academic Press Wilson, T. and Gilbert, D. (2008). Explaining away: A model of affective adaptation, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5): 370-386 Wilson, T. Wheatley, T., Meyers, J., Gilbert, D., and Axsom, D. (2000). Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5): 821-836 Wirtz, D., Kruger, J., Scollon, C., Diener, E. (2003). What to do on Spring Break? The role of predicted, on-line, and remembered experience in future choice, Psychological Science, 14(5): 520-524 |
URI: | https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/id/eprint/44366 |