Da Silva, Sergio and De Faveri, Dinorá and Correa, Ana and Matsushita, Raul (2017): Social preferences, financial literacy and intertemporal choice.
Preview |
PDF
MPRA_paper_79535.pdf Download (1MB) | Preview |
Abstract
We evaluate the relationship between social preferences, financial literacy and intertemporal choice in questionnaires distributed to university students. Most respondents perform well on a financial literacy test, and the majority show prosocial value orientation. Older students tend to be more prosocial, but we cannot confirm in our sample that females are more prosocial than males. We cannot confirm, either, that the prosocial are more financially literate than individualists and the competitive. Most respondents do not show hyperbolic discounting, and its incidence abates as both stakes and payoff delays increase. Prosocial participants also reveal to be more patient across the questionnaires.
Item Type: | MPRA Paper |
---|---|
Original Title: | Social preferences, financial literacy and intertemporal choice |
Language: | English |
Keywords: | Social preferences, Social Value Orientation, Financial literacy, Intertemporal choice, Hyperbolic discounting, Impatience |
Subjects: | D - Microeconomics > D0 - General > D03 - Behavioral Microeconomics: Underlying Principles |
Item ID: | 79535 |
Depositing User: | Sergio Da Silva |
Date Deposited: | 06 Jun 2017 04:48 |
Last Modified: | 27 Sep 2019 07:09 |
References: | [1] Jamison J, Wegener J (2010) Multiple selves in intertemporal choice, Journal of Economic Psychology 31 (5) 832-839. [2] Da Silva S, Matsushita R, De Carvalho M (2015) Prosocial people take better care of their own future well-being, Open Access Library Journal 2, e2181. [3] Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim ASN (2009) The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 (3) 489-510. [4] Murphy RO, Ackermann KA, Handgraaf MJJ (2011) Measuring social value orientation, Judgment and Decision Making 6 (8), 771-781. [5] Murphy RO, Ackermann KA (2011) A review of measurement methods for social preferences, ETH Zurich Chair of Decision Theory and Behavioral Game Theory Working Paper. [6] Nax HH, Murphy RO, Ackermann KA (2015) Interactive preferences, Economics Letters 135 (3), 133-136. [7] De Dreu CKW, Boles TL (1998) Share and share alike or winner take all?: The influence of social value orientation upon choice and recall of negotiation heuristics, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 76 (3), 253-276. [8] Roch SG, Lane JAS, Samuelson CD, Allison ST, Dent JL (2000) Cognitive load and the equality heuristic: A two-stage model of resource overconsumption in small groups, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 83 (2), 185-212. [9] Zeelenberg M, Nelissen RMA, Breugelmans SM, Pieters R (2008) On emotion specificity in decision making: Why feeling is for doing, Judgment and Decision Making 3 (1), 18-27. [10] Van Lange PAM, De Bruin EMN, Otten W, Joireman JA (1997) Development of prosocial, individualistic, and competitive orientations: Theory and preliminary evidence, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73 (4), 733-746. [11] Lusardi A, Mitchell OS (2011) Financial literacy and planning: Implications for retirement wellbeing, NBER Working Paper No. 17078. [12] Moreira B, Matsushita R, Da Silva S (2010) Risk seeking behavior of preschool children in a gambling task. Journal of Economic Psychology 31 (5), 794-801. [13] Campbell BC, Dreber A, Apicella CL, Eisenberg DT, Gray PB, Little AC, Garcia JR, Zamore RS, Lum JK (2010) Testosterone exposure, dopaminergic reward, and sensation-seeking in young men. Physiology & Behavior 99 (4), 451-456. [14] Eckel CC, Grossman PJ (2008) Men, women and risk aversion: Experimental evidence. In: Plott C, Smith V (eds.), Handbook of Experimental Economics Results, Vol. 1. New York: Elsevier, pp. 1061-1073. [15] Da Silva S, De Faveri D, Correa A, Matsushita R (2017) High-income consumers may be less hyperbolic when discounting the future, unpublished. [16] Sutter M, Kocher MG, Glätzle-Rüetzler D, Trautmann ST (2013) Impatience and uncertainty: Experimental decisions predict adolescents’ field behavior, American Economic Review 103 (1), 510-531. [17] Liebrand WBG, McClintock CG (1988) The ring measure of social values: A computerized procedure for assessing individual differences in information processing and social value orientation, European Journal of Personality 2 (3), 217-230. [18] Liebrand WBG (1984) The effect of social motives, communication and group size on behaviour in an n-person multi-stage mixed-motive game, European Journal of Social Psychology 14 (3), 239-264. [19] Balliet D, Joireman J (2010) Ego depletion reduces proselfs’ concern with the well-being of others, Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 13 (2), 227-239. [20] Frederick S, Loewenstein G, O’Donoghue T (2002) Time discounting and time preference: A critical review, Journal of Economic Literature 40 (2), 351-401. [21] Prelec D, Loewenstein G (1991) Decision making over time and under uncertainty: A common approach, Management Science 37 (7), 770-786. [22] Bartels DM, Urminsky O (2011) On intertemporal selfishness: How the perceived instability of identity underlies impatient consumption, Journal of Consumer Research 38 (1), 182-198. |
URI: | https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/id/eprint/79535 |