Chen, Chia-Ching and Chiu, I-Ming and Smith, John and Yamada, Tetsuji (2012): Too smart to be selfish? Measures of cognitive ability, social preferences, and consistency.
Preview |
PDF
MPRA_paper_41078.pdf Download (192kB) | Preview |
Abstract
Although there is an increasing interest in examining the relationship between cognitive ability and economic behavior, less is known about the relationship between cognitive ability and social preferences. We investigate the relationship between consequential measures of cognitive ability and measures of social preferences. We have data on a series of small-stakes dictator-type decisions, known as Social Value Orientation (SVO), in addition to choices in a larger-stakes dictator game. We also have access to the grade point averages (GPA) and SAT (formerly referred to as the Scholastic Aptitude Test) outcomes of our subjects. We find that subjects who perform better on the Math portion of the SAT are more generous in both the dictator game and the SVO measure. By contrast we find that subjects with a higher GPA are more selfish in the dictator game and more generous according to the SVO. We also find some evidence that the subjects with higher GPA and higher SAT outcomes offer more consistent responses. Our results involving GPA and social preferences complement previous work which employ measures of cognitive ability which are sensitive to the intrinsic motivation of the subject. Our results involving SAT scores are without precedent in the literature and suggest that measures of cognitive ability, which are less sensitive to the intrinsic motivation of the subject, are positively related to generosity.
Item Type: | MPRA Paper |
---|---|
Original Title: | Too smart to be selfish? Measures of cognitive ability, social preferences, and consistency |
Language: | English |
Keywords: | dictator game; Social Value Orientation; altruism; intelligence |
Subjects: | D - Microeconomics > D6 - Welfare Economics > D64 - Altruism ; Philanthropy C - Mathematical and Quantitative Methods > C9 - Design of Experiments > C91 - Laboratory, Individual Behavior |
Item ID: | 41078 |
Depositing User: | John Smith |
Date Deposited: | 07 Sep 2012 10:14 |
Last Modified: | 27 Sep 2019 16:54 |
References: | Andreoni, James and Vesterlund, Lise (2001): "Which is the Fair Sex? Gender Differences in Altruism," Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1), 293--312. Arruñada, Benito, Casari, Marco, and Pancotto, Francesca (2012): "Are Self-regarding Subjects More Rational?" working paper Universita' di Bologna. Ballinger, T. Parker, Hudson, Eric, Karkoviata, Leonie, and Wilcox, Nathaniel T. (2011): "Saving behavior and cognitive abilities," Experimental Economics, 14, 349--374. Bayer, Ralph and Renou, Ludovic (2011): "Logical abilities and behavior in strategic-form games," working paper University of Leicester. Beaujean, A. Alexander, Firmin, Michael W., Knoop, Andrew J., Michonski, Jared D., Berry, Theodore P., and Lowrie, Ruth E. (2006): "Validation of the Frey and Detterman (2004) IQ prediction equations using the Reynolds Intellectual Assessment Scales," Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 353--357. Bednar, Jenna, Chen, Yan, Liu, Tracy Xiao, and Page, Scott (2012): "Behavioral Spillovers and Cognitive Load in Multiple Games: An Experimental Study," Games and Economic Behavior, 74(1), 12--31 Benjamin, Daniel, Brown, Sebastian, and Shapiro, Jesse (2012): "Who is 'Behavioral?' Cognitive Ability and Anomalous Preferences," Journal of the European Economic Association, forthcoming. Ben-Ner, Avner, Kong, Fanmin and Putterman, Louis (2004): "Share and share alike? Gender-pairing, personality, and cognitive ability as determinants of giving," Journal of Economic Psychology, 25, 581--589. Bergman, Oscar, Ellingsen, Tore, Johannesson, Magnus, and Svensson, Cicek (2010): "Anchoring and cognitive ability," Economics Letters, 107, 66--68. Borghans, Lex, Meijers, Huub, and Ter Weel, Bas (2008): "The Role of Nongognitive Skills in Explaining Cognitive Test Scores," Economic Inquiry, 46(1), 2--12. Brandstätter, Hernann and Güth, Werner (2002): "Personality in Dictator and Ultimatum Games," Central European Journal of Operations Research, 10(3), 191--215. Brañas-Garza, Pablo, Garcia-Muñoz, Teresa and Hernan Gonzalez, Roberto (2012): "Cognitive effort in the Beauty Contest Game," Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 83(2): 254--260. Brañas-Garza, Pablo, Guillen, Pablo and Lopez del Paso, Rafael (2008): "Math skills and risk attitudes," Economics Letters, 99, 332--336. Brañas-Garza, Pablo, Paz Espinosa, Maria and Rey-Biel, Pedro (2011): "Travelers' types," Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 78, 25--36. Burks, Stephen V., Carpenter, Jeffrey P., Götte, Lorenz and Rustichini, Aldo (2009): "Cognitive Skills Explain Economic Preferences, Strategic Behavior, and Job Attachment," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(19), 7745--7750. Burnham, Terence C., Cesarini, David, Johannesson, Magnus, Lichtenstein, Paul and Wallace, Björn (2009): "Higher cognitive ability is associated with lower entries in a p-beauty contest," Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 72, 171--175. Camerer, Colin F., Ho, Teck-Hua, and Chong, Juin Kuan (2004): "A Cognitive Hierarchy Model of Games," Quarterly Journal of Economics, 119(3), 861--898. Camerer, Colin F., and Hogarth, Robin M. (1999): " The effects of financial incentives in experiments: a review and capital-labor-production framework," Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19(1-3), 7--42. Carpenter, Jeffrey (2005): "Endogenous Social Preferences," Review of Radical Political Economics, 37(1): 63--84. Charness, Gary and Rabin, Matthew (2002): "Understanding Social Preferences with Simple Tests," Quarterly Journal of Economics, 117(3), 817--869. Cokely, Edward T. and Kelley, Colleen M. (2009): "Cognitive abilities and superior decision making under risk: A protocol analysis and process model evaluation," Judgment and Decision Making, 4(1), 20--33. Cornelissen, Gert, Dewitte, Siegfried, and Warlop, Luk (2011): "Are Social Value Orientations expressed automatically? Decision making in the dictator game," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(8), 1080--1090. Costa-Gomes, Miguel A., Crawford, Vincent P. and Broseta, Bruno (2001): "Cognition and Behavior in Normal-Form Games: An Experimental Study," Econometrica, 69(5), 1193--1235. Coyle, Thomas R. and Pillow, David R. (2008): "SAT and ACT predict college GPA after removing g," Intelligence, 36, 719--729. Coyle, Thomas, Snyder, Anissa, Pillow, David, and Kochunov, Peter (2011): "SAT predicts GPA better for high ability subjects: Implications for Spearman'sLaw of Diminishing Returns," Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 470--474. Devetag, Giovanna and Warglien, Massimo (2003): "Games and phone numbers: Do short-term memory bounds affect strategic behavior?" Journal of Economic Psychology, 24, 189--202. Dickinson, David L., and McElroy, Todd (2010): "Rationality around the clock: Sleep and time-of-day effects on guessing game responses," Economics Letters, 108, 245--248. Dohmen, Thomas, Falk, Armin, Huffman, David, and Sunde, Uwe (2010): "Are Risk Aversion and Impatience Related to Cognitive Ability?" American Economic Review, 100(3), 1238--1260. Duckworth, Angela Lee, Quinn, Patrick D., Lynam, Donald R., Loeber, Rolf, and Stouthamer-Loeber, Magda (2011): "Role of test motivation in intelligence testing," Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(19), 7716--7720. Duckworth, Angela L. and Seligman, Martin E.P. (2005): "Self-Discipline Outdoes IQ in Predicting Academic Performance of Adolescents," Psychological Science, 16(14), 939--944. Duffy, Sean and Smith, John (2012): "Cognitive Load in the Multi-player Prisoner's Dilemma Game: Are There Brains in Games?" working paper Rutgers University-Camden. Eckel, Catherine (1999): "Commentary on 'The Effects of Financial Incentives in Experiments: A Review and Capital-Labor-Production Framework'," Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19, 47--48. Eckel, Catherine and Grossman, Philip (1998): "Are Women Less Selfish Than Men? Evidence from Dictator Experiments," Economic Journal, 108(448), 726--735. Frederick, Shane (2005): "Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making," Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25--42. Frey, Meredith C. and Detterman, Douglas K. (2004): "Scholastic Assessment or g? The Relationship between the Scholastic Assessment Test and General Cognitive Ability," Psychological Science, 15(6), 373-378. Georganas, Sotiris, Healy, Paul J., and Weber, Roberto A. (2010): "On the Persistence of Strategic Sophistication," working paper Ohio State and Carnegie Mellon University. Gill, David and Prowse, Victoria (2012): "Cognitive ability and learning to play equilibrium: A level-k analysis," working paper Oxford University and Cornell University. Hauge, Karen Evelyn, Brekke, Kjell Arne, Johansson, Lars-Olof, Johansson-Stenman, Olof, and Svedsäter, Henrik (2009): "Are Social Preferences Skin Deep? Dictators under Cognitive Load," working paper University of Gothenburg. Hoogendoorn, Sander, Parker, Simon, and van Praag, Mirjam (2012): "Ability dispersion and team performance: a randomized field experiment," working paper University of Amsterdam. Hoppe, Eva I. and Kusterer, David J. (2011): "Behavioral biases and cognitive reflection," Economics Letters, 110, 97--100. James, Russell N. (2011): "Charitable giving and cognitive ability," International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, 16, 70--83. Jones, Garett (2008): "Are smarter groups more cooperative? Evidence from prisoner's dilemma experiments, 1959-2003," Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 68, 489--497. Jones, Matthew T. (2011): "Strategic Complexity and Cooperation: An Experimental Study," working paper, Ohio State University. Kappe, Rutger and van der Flier, Henk (2012): "Predicting academic success in higher education: what's more important than being smart?" European Journal of Psychology of Education, forthcoming. Kirby, Kris N., Winston, Gordon C., and Santiesteban, Mariana (2005): "Impatience and grades: Delay-discount rates correlate negatively with college GPA," Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 213--222. Liberali, Jordana, Reyna, Valerie, Furlan, Sarah, Stein, Lilian, and Pardo, Seth (2012): "Individual Differences in Numeracy and Cognitive Reflection, with Implications for Biases and Fallacies in Probability Judgment," Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(4): 361--381. Millet, Kobe and Dewitte, Siegfried (2007): "Altruistic Behavior as a Costly Signal of General Intelligence," Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 316-326. Nagel, Rosemarie (1995): "Unraveling in Guessing Games: An Experimental Study," American Economic Review, 85(5), 1313--1326. Noftle, Erik E. and Robins, Richard W. (2007): "Personality Predictors of Academic Outcomes: Big Five Correlates of GPA and SAT Scores," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(1), 116--130. Oechssler, Jörg, Roider, Andreas and Schmitz, Patrick W. (2009): "Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases," Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 72, 147--152. Palacios-Huerta, Ignacio (2003) "Learning to Open Monty Hall's Doors," Experimental Economics, 6(3), 235--251. Putterman, Louis, Tyran, Jean-Robert and Kamei, Kenju (2011): "Public goods and voting on formal sanction schemes," Journal of Public Economics, 95, 1213--1222. Roch, Sylvia G., Lane, John A. S., Samuelson, Charles D., Allison, Scott T. and Dent, Jennifer L. (2000): "Cognitive Load and the Equality Heuristic: A Two-Stage Model of Resource Overconsumption in Small Groups," Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 83(2), 185--212. Rydval, Ondrej (2011): "The Causal Effect of Cognitive Abilities on Economic Behavior: Evidence from a Forecasting Task with Varying Cognitive Load," working paper CERGE-EI. Rydval, Ondrej and Ortmann, Andreas (2004): "How financial incentives and cognitive abilities affect task performance in laboratory settings: an illustration," Economics Letters, 85, 315--320. Savikhin, Anya and Sheremeta, Roman M. (2012): "Simultaneous Decision-Making in Competitive and Cooperative Environments," Economic Inquiry, forthcoming. Schnusenberg, Oliver and Gallo, Andrés (2011): "On cognitive ability and learning in a beauty contest," Journal for Economic Educators, 11(1), 13--24. Segal, Carmit (2012): "Working When No One Is Watching: Motivation, Test Scores, and Economic Success," Management Science, forthcoming. Schulz, Jonathan F., Fischbacher, Urs, Thöni, Christian, Utikal, Verena (2011): "Affect and fairness: Dictator games under cognitive load," working paper #68, Thurgau Institute of Economics and Department of Economics at the University of Konstanz. Smith, John (2012): "The Endogenous Nature of the Measurement of Social Preferences," Mind and Society, forthcoming. Stanovich, Keith E. and West, Richard F. (2008): "On the Relative Independence of Thinking Biases and Cognitive Ability," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(4), 672--695. Swope, Kurtis J., Cadigan, John, Schmitt, Pamela M., and Shupp, Robert (2008): "Personality preferences in laboratory economics experiments," Journal of Socio-Economics, 37, 998--1009. Thöni, Christian , Tyran, Jean-Robert and Wengström, Erik (2012): "Microfoundations of Social Capital," Journal of Public Economics, 96(7-8): 635--643. Van Lange, Paul A.M., Otten, Wilma, De Bruin, Ellen M.N. and Joireman, Jeffrey (1997): "Development of Prosocial, Individualistic, and Competitive Orientations: Theory and Preliminary Evidence," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(4), 733--746. Visser, Michael S. and Roelofs, Matthew R. (2011): "Heterogeneous preferences for altruism: gender and personality, social status, giving and taking," Experimental Economics, 14(4), 490--506. Yang, Bijou and Lester, David (2008): "Reflections on rational choice: The existence of systematic irrationality," Journal of Socio-Economics, 37, 1218--1233. |
URI: | https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/id/eprint/41078 |