Allred, Sarah and Crawford, L. Elizabeth and Duffy, Sean and Smith, John (2014): Cognitive constraints increase estimation biases: Cognitive load and delay in judgments.
Preview |
PDF
MPRA_paper_58314.pdf Download (256kB) | Preview |
Abstract
Previous work has demonstrated that memory for simple stimuli can be biased by information about the category of which the stimulus is a member. These biases have been interpreted as optimally integrating noisy sensory information with category information. A separate literature has demonstrated that cognitive load can lead to biases in social cognition. Here we link the two, asking whether delay (Experiment 1) and cognitive load (Experiment 2) affect the extent to which observers' memories for simple line stimuli are affected by category information. We found that delay and cognitive load have similar effects: both manipulations increase the weight of category information on memory for stimuli. We discuss the broad implications of such findings on fields such as eyewitness testimony.
Item Type: | MPRA Paper |
---|---|
Original Title: | Cognitive constraints increase estimation biases: Cognitive load and delay in judgments |
Language: | English |
Keywords: | cognitive load; delay; judgment; estimation biases; memory; category effects |
Subjects: | C - Mathematical and Quantitative Methods > C9 - Design of Experiments > C91 - Laboratory, Individual Behavior |
Item ID: | 58314 |
Depositing User: | John Smith |
Date Deposited: | 06 Sep 2014 10:09 |
Last Modified: | 27 Sep 2019 16:53 |
References: | Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification, IEEE Trans. Automatic Control 19, 716–723. DOI: 10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705 Allred, S., Duffy, S., & Smith, J. (under revision) Cognitive load and strategic sophistication. Ashourian, P., & Loewenstein, Y. (2011). Bayesian inference underlies the contraction bias in delayed comparison tasks. PloS One, 6(5), e19551. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0019551 Bergman, O., Ellingsen, T., Johannesson, M., & Svensson, C. (2010). Anchoring and cognitive ability. Economics Letters, 107, 66–68. DOI: 10.1016/j.econlet.2009.12.028 Christianson, S. Å. (1992). Emotional stress and eyewitness memory: a critical review. Psychological bulletin, 112, 284-309. Cornelissen, G., Dewitte, S., and Warlop, L. (2011). Are social value orientations expressed automatically? Decision making in the dictator game, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1080--1090. Crawford, L.E., & Duffy, S. (2010). Sequence effects in estimating spatial location. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15, 576-588. DOI:10.3758/PBR.17.2.224 Crawford, L.E., Duffy, S., & Falcone, D. (in preparation). Effect of estimating stimuli on inductive category formation. Crawford, L. E., Huttenlocher, J., & Engebretson, P. H. (2000). Category effects on estimates of stimuli: Perception or reconstruction. Psychological Science, 11(4), 280-284. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00256 Crawford, L. E., Huttenlocher, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2006). Within-category feature correlations and Bayesian adjustment strategies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(2), 245-250. DOI: 10.3758/BF03193838 Duffy, S., & Crawford, L.E. (2008). Primacy or recency effects in the formation of inductive categories. Memory and Cognition, 36, 567-577. DOI:10.3758/MC.36.3.567 Duffy, S., & Kitayama, S. (2007). Mnemonic context effect in two cultures: An examination of culturally contingent attention strategies. Cognitive Science, 31, 1009-1020. DOI: 10.1080/03640210701703808 Duffy, S. & Smith, J. (2014). Are there brains in games? Cognitive load in the multiple player prisoner’s dilemma game. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 47-56. DOI: 10.1016/j.socec.2014.01.006 Duffy, S., Huttenlocher, J., & Crawford, L. E. (2006). Children use categories to maximize accuracy in estimation. Developmental Science, 9(6), 597-603. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00538.x Duffy, S., Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Crawford, L. E. (2010). Category effects on stimulus estimation: Shifting and skewed frequency distributions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 224-230. DOI:10.3758/PBR.17.2.224 Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of personality and social psychology, 54, 733. Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L., and Whitney, P. (2003). Impulsive decision making and working memory, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 298--306. Hollingworth, H. L. (1910). The central tendency of judgment. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 7, 461-468. DOI:10.2307/2012819 Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Duncan, S. (1991). Categories and particulars: prototype effects in estimating spatial location. Psychological Review, 98(3), 352. Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (2000). Why do categories affect stimulus judgment? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 220-241. Magnussen, S., & Greenlee, M. W. (1999). The psychophysics of perceptual memory. Psychological Research, 62, 81–92. DOI:10.1007/s004260050043 Jazayeri, M., & Shadlen, M. N. (2010). Temporal context calibrates interval timing. Nature Neuroscience, 13(8), 1020–6. DOI:10.1038/nn.2590 Mani, A., Mullainathan, S., Shafir, E., & Zhao, J. (2013). Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science, 341, 976-980. DOI:10.1126/science.1238041 Mann, T., & Ward, A. (2007). Attention, self-control, and health behaviors. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 280-283. Downloaded from http://www.jstor.org.proxy.libraries.rutgers.edu/stable/20183216 Oechssler, J., Roider, A. and Schmitz, P. W. (2009) Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 72, 147–152. DOI: 10.1016/j.jebo.2009.04.018 Olkkonen, M., & Allred, S. R. (2014). Short-Term Memory Affects Color Perception in Context. PLoS ONE, 9(1), e86488. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0086488 Olkkonen, M. McCarthy, P. F, & Allred, S.R (in press). The central tendency bias in color perception: effects of internal and external noise. Journal of Vision. Shiv, B. and Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making, Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 278-292. Swann, W. B., Hixon, G., Stein-Seroussi, A., and Gilbert, D. T. (1990). The fleeting gleam of praise: Cognitive processes underlying behavioral reactions to self-relevant feedback," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 17-26. Van den Bos, K., Peters, S. L., Bobocel, D. R., and Ybema, J. F. (2006) On preferences and doing the right thing: Satisfaction with advantageous inequity when cognitive processing is limited. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 273--289. Van Knippenberg, A. D., Dijksterhuis, A. P., & Vermeulen, D. (1999). Judgement and memory of a criminal act: The effects of stereotypes and cognitive load. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 191-201. DOI:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199903/05)29:2/3<191 Wigboldus, D. H., Sherman, J. W., Franzese, H. L., & Knippenberg, A. V. (2004). Capacity and comprehension: Spontaneous stereotyping under cognitive load. Social Cognition, 22(3), 292-309. DOI:10.1177/02729 |
URI: | https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/id/eprint/58314 |