Colasante, Annarita and Marini, Matteo M. and Russo, Alberto (2017): Incidental emotions and risk-taking: An experimental analysis.
This is the latest version of this item.
Preview |
PDF
MPRA_paper_82767.pdf Download (303kB) | Preview |
Abstract
In this paper we run a laboratory experiment in order to investigate the impact of incidental emotions on individual risk-taking. In particular, we induce sadness and happiness by means of audiovisual stimuli and compare the subsequent risky choices with the baseline thanks to a between-subjects design. A tweaked version of the Multiple Price List method is used to elicit individual risk preferences in the context of three different lotteries. As main result, the experimental subjects exhibit greater risk aversion under sadness or happiness, than under neutral conditions. Therefore, we explain the fndings through the theory of ego depletion, whereby controlling emotions so as to subsequently process information consumes a limited self-regulatory resource, which is necessary to take risks as well. The outcome is detected in the first lottery but not from the second lottery onwards, probably due to unchecked order effect.
Item Type: | MPRA Paper |
---|---|
Original Title: | Incidental emotions and risk-taking: An experimental analysis |
Language: | English |
Keywords: | laboratory experiment, music, mood induction, preference elicitation, risk aversion. |
Subjects: | C - Mathematical and Quantitative Methods > C9 - Design of Experiments > C91 - Laboratory, Individual Behavior D - Microeconomics > D8 - Information, Knowledge, and Uncertainty > D81 - Criteria for Decision-Making under Risk and Uncertainty |
Item ID: | 82767 |
Depositing User: | Alberto Russo |
Date Deposited: | 17 Nov 2017 15:37 |
Last Modified: | 26 Sep 2019 23:58 |
References: | Abdellaoui, M. (2000). Parameter-free elicitation of utility and probability weighting functions. Management Science, 46(11):1497-1512. Abdellaoui, M., Driouchi, A., and L'Haridon, O. (2011). Risk aversion elicitation: reconciling tractability and bias minimization. Theory and Decision, 71(1):63-80. Andersen, S., Harrison, G. W., Lau, M. I., and Rutstrom, E. E. (2006). Elicitation using multiple price list formats. Experimental Economics, 9(4):383-405. Andrade, E. B. and Ariely, D. (2009). The enduring impact of transient emotions on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109(1):1-8. Baumeister, R. F., Bratlavsky, E., Muraven, M., and Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5):1252-1265. Binswanger, H. P. (1980). Attitudes toward risk: Experimental measurement in rural india. American Journal of Agricultural Economics, 62(3):395-407. Brandstatter, E., Kuhberger, A., and Schneider, F. (2002). A cognitive-emotional account of the shape of the probability weighting function. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(2):79-100. Campos-Vazquez, R. M. and Cuilty, E. (2014). The role of emotions on risk aversion: a prospect theory experiment. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 50:1-9. Charness, G., Gneezy, U., and Imas, A. (2013). Experimental methods: Eliciting risk preferences. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 87:43-51. Chou, K.-L., Lee, T., and Ho, A. H. (2007). Does mood state change risk taking tendency in older adults? Psychology and Aging, 22(2):310-318. Chuang, S.-C. and Lin, H.-M. (2007). The effect of induced positive and negative emotion and openess-to-feeling in student's consumer decision making. Journal of Business and Psychology, 22(1):65-78. Conte, A., Levati, M. V., and Nardi, C. (2016). Risk preferences and the role of emotions. Economica, forthcoming. Ding, X., Hartog, J., and Sun, Y. (2010). Can we measure individual risk attitudes in a survey? Technical report, Tinbergen Institute Discussion Paper. Drichoutis, A. C. and Nayga Jr, R. M. (2013). Eliciting risk and time preferences under induced mood states. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 45:18-27. Fischer, P., Greitemeyer, T., and Frey, D. (2007). Ego depletion and positive illusions: Does the construction of positivity require regulatory resources? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(9):1306-1321. Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117(1):39-66. Gneezy, U. and Potters, J. (1997). An experiment on risk taking and evaluation periods. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, pages 631-645. Holt, C. A. and Laury, S. K. (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review, 92(5):1644-1655. Isen, A. M. and Patrick, R. (1983). The effect of positive feelings on risk taking: When the chips are down. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 31(2):194-202. Jacobson, S. and Petrie, R. (2009). Learning from mistakes: What do inconsistent choices over risk tell us? Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 38(2):143-158. Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Oatley, K. (2008). Emotions, music, and literature. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., and Barrett, L. F., editors, Handbook of Emotions 3rd Edition. The Guilford Press. Kahneman, D. and Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In Gilovich, T., Griffn, D., and Kahneman, D., editors, Heuristics & biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. New York: Cambridge University Press. Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2):263-292. Kliger, D. and Levy, O. (2003). Mood-induced variation in risk preferences. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 52(4):573-584. Lejuez, C., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., Strong, D. R., and Brown, R. A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the balloon analogue risk task (bart). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(2):75-84. Lerner, J. S. and Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1):146-159. Loewenstein, G. and Lerner, J. S. (2003). The role of affect in decision making. Handbook of Affective Science, 619-642. Loewenstein, G., O'Donoghue, T., and Bhatia, S. (2015). Modeling the interplay between affect and deliberation. Decision, 2(2):55-81. Matell, M. and Jacoby, J. (1971). Is there an optimal number of alternatives for likert scale items? Educational and Psychological Measurement, 31:657-674. Mayer, J. D., Allen, I. P., and Beauregard, K. (1995). Mood inductions for four specific moods: A procedure employing guided imagery. Journal of Mental Imagery, 19(1-2):133-150. Nygren, T. E., Isen, A. M., Taylor, P. J., and Dulin, J. (1996). The influence of positive affect on the decision rule in risk situations: Focus on outcome (and especially avoidance of loss) rather than probability. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66(1):59-72. Plott, C. R. and Zeiler, K. (2005). The willingness to pay-willingness to accept gap, the. The American Economic Review, 95(3):530-545. Raghunathan, R. and Pham, M. T. (1999). All negative moods are not equal: Motivational in influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79(1):56-77. Rick, S. and Loewenstein, G. (2008). The role of emotion in economic behavior. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., and Barrett, L. F., editors, Handbook of Emotions 3rd Edition. The Guilford Press. Robbins, S. P. and Judge, T. A. (2012). Emotions and moods. In Robbins, S. P. and Judge, T. A., editors, Organizational Behavior 15th Edition. Prentice Hall. Shupp, R., Sheremeta, R. M., Schmidt, D., and Walker, J. (2013). Resource allocation contests: Experimental evidence. Journal of Economic Psychology, 39:257-267. Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5(4):297-323. Unger, A. and Stahlberg, D. (2011). Ego-depletion and risk behavior: Too exhausted to take a risk. Social Psychology, 42(1):28-38. Vastfjall, D. (2002). Emotion induction through music: A review of the musical mood induction procedure. Musicae Scientiae, Special issue 2001-2002:173-211. Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., and Hesse, F. W. (1996). Relative efectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26(4):557-580. Zeelenberg, M. and Pieters, R. (2006). Feeling is for doing: A pragmatic approach to the study of emotions in economic behavior. In DeCremer, D., Zeelenberg, M., and Murnighan, J. K., editors, Social Psychology and Economics. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Zizzo, D. J. (2010). Experimenter demand effects in economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 13:75-98. |
URI: | https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/id/eprint/82767 |
Available Versions of this Item
-
Incidental emotions and risk-taking: An experimental analysis. (deposited 22 Feb 2017 17:22)
- Incidental emotions and risk-taking: An experimental analysis. (deposited 17 Nov 2017 15:37) [Currently Displayed]