Börjesson, Maria and Fosgerau, Mogens (2015): Response Time Patterns in a Stated Choice Experiment. Forthcoming in: Journal of Choice Modelling
Preview |
PDF
MPRA_paper_62002.pdf Download (211kB) | Preview |
Abstract
This paper studies how response times vary between unlabeled binary choice occasions in a stated choice (SC) experiment, with alternatives differing with respect to in-vehicle travel time and travel cost. The pattern of response times is interpreted as an indicator of the cognitive processes employed by the respondents when making their choices. We find clear signs of reference-dependence in response times in the form of a strong gain-loss asymmetry. Moreover, different patterns of response times for travel time and travel cost indicate that these attributes are processed in different ways by respondents. This may be of particular relevance for choice experiments in the transportation field, where the travel time attribute is central.
Item Type: | MPRA Paper |
---|---|
Original Title: | Response Time Patterns in a Stated Choice Experiment |
Language: | English |
Keywords: | Response Times; Stated Choice; Data collection; Value of time |
Subjects: | C - Mathematical and Quantitative Methods > C8 - Data Collection and Data Estimation Methodology ; Computer Programs > C83 - Survey Methods ; Sampling Methods D - Microeconomics > D8 - Information, Knowledge, and Uncertainty > D87 - Neuroeconomics R - Urban, Rural, Regional, Real Estate, and Transportation Economics > R4 - Transportation Economics > R41 - Transportation: Demand, Supply, and Congestion ; Travel Time ; Safety and Accidents ; Transportation Noise |
Item ID: | 62002 |
Depositing User: | Prof. Mogens Fosgerau |
Date Deposited: | 11 Feb 2015 14:25 |
Last Modified: | 04 Oct 2019 16:50 |
References: | Bates, J., & Whelan, G. (2001). Size and sign of time savings. Working Paper. Institute of Transport Studies, University of Leeds , Leeds, UK. Retrieved from http://www.its.leeds.ac.uk/ Börjesson, M., & Eliasson, J. (2014). Experiences from the Swedish Value of Time study. Transportation Research A, 59, 144–158. Cantillo, V., Heydecker, B., & Ortúzar, J. de D. (2006). A discrete choice model incorporating thresholds for perception in attribute values. Transportation Research Part B: Methodological, 40(9), 807–825. doi:10.1016/j.trb.2005.11.002 Daly, A., Tsang, F., & Rohr, C. (2011). The value of small time savings for non-business travel. Proceedings from the European Transport Conference, 2011. De Borger, B., & Fosgerau, M. (2008). The trade-off between money and travel time: A test of the theory of reference-dependent preferences. Journal of Urban Economics, 64(1), 101–115. doi:16/j.jue.2007.09.001 Hess, S., & Rose, J. M. (2012). Can scale and coefficient heterogeneity be separated in random coefficients models? Transportation, 39(6), 1225–1239. doi:10.1007/s11116-012-9394-9 Hess, S., & Stathopoulos, A. (2013). Linking response quality to survey engagement: A combined random scale and latent variable approach. Journal of Choice Modelling, 7, 1–12. doi:10.1016/j.jocm.2013.03.005 Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 133–168). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. Hjorth, K., & Fosgerau, M. (2011). Loss Aversion and Individual Characteristics. Environmental and Resource Economics, 49(4), 573–596. doi:10.1007/s10640-010-9455-5 Hultkrantz, L., Mortazavi, R., & Hulkranz, L. (2001). Anomalies in the Value of Travel-Time Changes. Journal of Transport Economics and Policy, 35(2), 285–299. doi:10.2307/20053871 Kahneman, D. (2003). Maps of Bounded Rationality: Psychology for Behavioral Economics. The American Economic Review, 93(5), 1449–1475. Klein, N. M., & Yadav, M. S. (1989). Context Effects on Effort and Accuracy in Choice: An Enquiry into Adaptive Decision Making. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(4), 411–421. Louviere, J., & Eagle, T. (2006). Confound it! That pesky little scale constant messes up our convenient assumptions. Retrieved from http://epress.lib.uts.edu.au/research/handle/10453/7515 Louviere, J. J., Meyer, R. J., Bunch, D. S., Carson, R., Dellaert, B., Hanemann, W. M., … Irwin, J. (1999). Combining Sources of Preference Data for Modeling Complex Decision Processes. Marketing Letters, 10(3), 205–217. doi:10.1023/A:1008050215270 Louviere, J., Street, D., Carson, R., Ainslie, A., Deshazo, J. R., Cameron, T., … Marley, T. (2002). Dissecting the Random Component of Utility. Marketing Letters, 13(3), 177–193. doi:10.1023/A:1020258402210 Malhotra, N. (2008). Completion Time and Response Order Effects in Web Surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72(5), 914–934. doi:10.1093/poq/nfn050 Pagan, A., & Ullah, A. (1999). Nonparametric Econometrics. Cambridge University Press. Palmer, S. E. (1999). Vision science: Photons to phenomenology (Vol. xxii). Cambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press. Rose, J. M., & Black, I. R. (2006). Means matter, but variance matter too: Decomposing response latency influences on variance heterogeneity in stated preference experiments. Marketing Letters, 17(4), 295–310. doi:10.1007/s11002-006-8632-3 Susilo, Y. (2005). The Short-term Variability and the Long-term Changes of Individual Spatial Behavior in Urban Areas (PhD Thesis). Kyoto University. Swait, J., & Bernardino, A. (2000). Distinguishing taste variation from error structure in discrete choice data. Transportation Research Part B: Methodological, 34(1), 1–15. doi:10.1016/S0191-2615(99)00009-0 Tom, S. M., Fox, C. R., Trepel, C., & Poldrack, R. A. (2007). The Neural Basis of Loss Aversion in Decision-Making Under Risk. Science, 315(5811), 515–518. doi:10.1126/science.1134239 Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1991). Loss Aversion in Riskless Choice: A Reference-Dependent Model. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 106(4), 1039–1061. doi:10.2307/2937956 |
URI: | https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/id/eprint/62002 |