Da Silva, Sergio and Matsushita, Raul and De Carvalho, Mateus (2015): Prosocial People Take Better Care of Their Own Future Well-Being. Published in: Open Access Library Journal , Vol. 2, (2015): e2181.
Preview |
PDF
MPRA_paper_68452.pdf Download (516kB) | Preview |
Abstract
There is neuroscientific evidence that people consider future versions of themselves as other people. As a result, intertemporal choice should refer to the interaction between multiple selves. We combine this notion of multiple selves in delay discounting with the approach for other-regarding preferences known as Social Value Orientation. The Social Value Orientation is a psychologically richer framework that generalizes the economic assumption of narrow self-interest. People are assumed to vary in their motivations toward resource allocation between them and the others. When making such allocation decisions they may still be individualistic, but can also be competitive, prosocial, or even altruistic. We apply an experimental measure of impatience to a sample of 437 undergraduates, measure their Social Value Orientation, and collect selected demographic variables: gender, age, handedness, parenthood, religiousness, and current emotional state. We find prosocial participants to be more patient. Those who care for the others in the present also take better care of themselves in the future. We also find a participant’s age and handedness to matter for his or her Social Value Orientation.
Item Type: | MPRA Paper |
---|---|
Original Title: | Prosocial People Take Better Care of Their Own Future Well-Being |
Language: | English |
Keywords: | Other-Regarding Preferences, Social Value Orientation, Multiple Selves, Intertemporal Choice, Impatience |
Subjects: | D - Microeconomics > D0 - General > D03 - Behavioral Microeconomics: Underlying Principles D - Microeconomics > D6 - Welfare Economics > D64 - Altruism ; Philanthropy D - Microeconomics > D9 - Intertemporal Choice > D91 - Intertemporal Household Choice ; Life Cycle Models and Saving |
Item ID: | 68452 |
Depositing User: | Sergio Da Silva |
Date Deposited: | 20 Dec 2015 23:59 |
Last Modified: | 27 Sep 2019 17:51 |
References: | [1] Jamison, J. and Wegener, J. (2010) Multiple Selves in Intertemporal Choice. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31, 832-839. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2010.03.004 [2] Spreng, R.N., Mar, R.A. and Kim, A.S.N. (2009) The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-Analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 489-510. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.21029 [3] Sutter, M., Kocher, M.G., Glatzle-Ruetzler, D. and Trautmann, S.T. (2013) Impatience and Uncertainty: Experimental Decisions Predict Adolescents’ Field Behavior. American Economic Review, 103, 510-531. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.103.1.510 [4] Murphy, R.O., Ackermann, K.A. and Handgraaf, M.J.J. (2011) Measuring Social Value Orientation. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 771-781. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1804189 [5] Murphy, R.O. and Ackermann, K.A. (2011) A Review of Measurement Methods for Social Preferences. ETH Zurich Chair of Decision Theory and Behavioral Game Theory, Working Paper. [6] Llaurens, V., Raymond, M. and Faurie, C. (2009) Why Are Some People Left-Handed? An Evolutionary Perspective. Philosophical Transactions of the Real Society of London B, 364, 881-894. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0235 [7] Moreira, B., Matsushita, R. and Da Silva, S. (2010) Risk Seeking Behavior of Preschool Children in a Gambling Task. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31, 794-801. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2010.03.005 [8] Campbell, B.C., Dreber, A., Apicella, C.L., Eisenberg, D.T., Gray, P.B., Little, A.C., Garcia, J.R., Zamore, R.S. and Lum, J.K. (2010) Testosterone Exposure, Dopaminergic Reward, and Sensation-Seeking in Young Men. Physiology & Behavior, 99, 451-456. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.12.011 [9] Dohmen, T.J., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Sunde, U., Schupp, J. and Wagner, G.G. (2005) Individual Risk Attitudes: New Evidence from a Large, Representative, Experimentally Validated Survey. IZA Discussion Papers No. 1730. [10] Da Silva, S., Baldo, D. and Matsushita, R. (2013) Biological Correlates of the Allais Paradox. Applied Economics, 45, 555-568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00036846.2011.607133 [11] Liebrand, W.B.G. (1984) The Effect of Social Motives, Communication and Group Size on Behaviour in an N-Person Multi-Stage Mixed-Motive Game. European Journal of Social Psychology, 14, 239-264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420140302 [12] Van Lange, P.A.M., De Bruin, E.M.N., Otten, W. and Joireman, J.A. (1997) Development of Prosocial, Individualistic, and Competitive Orientations: Theory and Preliminary Evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 733-746. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.4.733 [13] Liebrand, W.B.G. and McClintock, C.G. (1988) The Ring Measure of Social Values: A Computerized Procedure for Assessing Individual Differences in Information Processing and Social Value Orientation. European Journal of Personality, 2, 217-230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.2410020304 |
URI: | https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/id/eprint/68452 |