Barron, Kai and Gravert, Christina (2018): Beliefs and actions: How a shift in confidence affects choices.
Preview |
PDF
MPRA_paper_84743.pdf Download (1MB) | Preview |
Abstract
Confidence is often seen as the key to success. Empirical evidence about whether such beliefs causally map into actions is, however, sparse. In this paper, we experimentally investigate the causal effect of an increase in confidence about one’s own ability on two central choices made by workers in the labor market: choosing between jobs with different incentive schemes, and the subsequent choice of how much effort to exert within the job. An increase in confidence leads to an increase in self-selection into uncertain ability-contingent payment schemes. This is detrimental for low ability workers. Policy implications are discussed.
Item Type: | MPRA Paper |
---|---|
Original Title: | Beliefs and actions: How a shift in confidence affects choices |
Language: | English |
Keywords: | Overconfidence; experiment; beliefs; real-effort; grade inflation |
Subjects: | C - Mathematical and Quantitative Methods > C9 - Design of Experiments > C91 - Laboratory, Individual Behavior D - Microeconomics > D0 - General > D03 - Behavioral Microeconomics: Underlying Principles J - Labor and Demographic Economics > J2 - Demand and Supply of Labor > J24 - Human Capital ; Skills ; Occupational Choice ; Labor Productivity M - Business Administration and Business Economics ; Marketing ; Accounting ; Personnel Economics > M5 - Personnel Economics > M50 - General |
Item ID: | 84743 |
Depositing User: | Dr Kai Barron |
Date Deposited: | 21 Feb 2018 15:13 |
Last Modified: | 29 Sep 2019 09:46 |
References: | Arcidiacono, P., E. M. Aucejo, and V. J. Hotz (2016, March). University differences in the graduation of minorities in stem fields: Evidence from california. American Economic Review 106(3), 525–62. Barber, B. M. and T. Odean (2001). Boys will be boys: Gender, overconfidence, and common stock investment. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 116(1), 261–292. B´enabou, R. and J. Tirole (2002). Self-confidence and personal motivation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 117(3), 871–915. Benoˆıt, J.-P., J. Dubra, and D. A. Moore (2015). Does the better-than-average effect show that people are overconfident?: Two experiments. Journal of the European Economic Association 13(2), 293–329. Bernardo, A. E. and I. Welch (2001). On the evolution of overconfidence and entrepreneurs. Journal of Economics & Management Strategy 10(3), 301–330. Bertrand, M. (2011). New perspectives on gender. Handbook of Labor Economics 4, 1543–1590. Bonin, H., T. Dohmen, A. Falk, D. Huffman, and U. Sunde (2007). Cross-sectional earnings risk and occupational sorting: The role of risk attitudes. Labour Economics 14(6), 926–937. Bordley, R., M. LiCalzi, and L. Tibiletti (2014). A target-based foundation for the hard-easy effect bias. Working Papers 23, Department of Management, Universit˜A Ca’ Foscari Venezia. Bruhin, A., L. Santos-Pinto, D. Staubli, et al. (2016). How do beliefs about skill affect risky decisions? Technical report. Burks, S. V., J. P. Carpenter, L. Goette, and A. Rustichini (2013). Overconfidence and social signalling. Review of Economic Studies 80(3), 949–983. Burson, K. A., R. P. Larrick, and J. Klayman (2006). Skilled or unskilled, but still unaware of it how perceptions of difficulty drive miscalibration in relative comparisons. Journal of personality and social psychology 90(1), 60. Cheung, S. L. and L. Johnstone (2017, February). True Overconfidence, Revealed through Actions: An Experiment. IZA Discussion Papers 10545, Institute for the Study of Labor (IZA). 39 Clarida, M. and N. Fandos (2013). Substantiating fears of grade inflation, dean says median grade at harvard college is a-, most common grade is a. Technical report. Corgnet, B., R. Hern´an-Gonz´alez, and E. Schniter (2015, Jun). Why real leisure really matters: incentive effects on real effort in the laboratory. Experimental Economics 18(2), 284–301. Costa-Gomes, M. A., S. Huck, and G. Weizs¨acker (2014). Beliefs and actions in the trust game: Creating instrumental variables to estimate the causal effect. Games and Economic Behavior 88, 298–309. Costa-Gomes, M. A. and G. Weizs¨acker (2008). Stated beliefs and play in normal-form games. The Review of Economic Studies 75(3), 729–762. Croson, R. and U. Gneezy (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature 47(2), 448–74. Dargnies, M.-P., R. Hakimov, and D. K¨ubler (2016). Self-confidence and unraveling in matching markets. WZB Working Paper Series. de Araujo, F. A., E. Carbone, L. Conell-Price, M. W. Dunietz, A. Jaroszewicz, R. Landsman, D. Lam´e, L. Vesterlund, S. Wang, and A. J. Wilson (2015). The effect of incentives of real effort: Evidence from the slider task. DellaVigna, S. and D. Pope (2016). What motivates effort? evidence and expert forecasts. The Review of Economic Studies. Eckel, C. C. and P. J. Grossman (2008). Chapter 113 men, women and risk aversion: Experimental evidence. Handbook of Experimental Economics Results 1, 1061 – 1073. Erkal, N., L. Gangadharan, and B. Han Koh (2016). What motivates participants in real-effort tasks: Is it the journey or the destination? Working Paper. Falk, A., A. Becker, T. Dohmen, D. Huffman, and U. Sunde (2016, February). The Preference Survey Module: A Validated Instrument for Measuring Risk, Time, and Social Preferences. Working Papers 2016-003, Human Capital and Economic Opportunity Working Group. Fischbacher, U. (2007). z-tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental economics 10(2), 171–178. 40 G¨achter, S., L. Huang, and M. Sefton (2016). Combining “real effort” with induced effort costs: the ball-catching task. Experimental economics 19(4), 687–712. Gill, D. and V. Prowse (2011). A novel computerized real effort task based on sliders. IZA Discussion Papers 5801, Institute for the Study of Labor (IZA). Gneezy, U., C. Gravert, S. Saccardo, and F. Tausch (2017). A must lie situation - avoiding giving negative feedback. Games and Economic Behavior 102, 445 – 454. Greiner, B. (2015). Subject pool recruitment procedures: organizing experiments with orsee. Journal of the Economic Science Association 1(1), 114–125. Griffith, E. (2014). Why startups fail, according to their founders. Technical report. Healy, P. J. and D. A. Moore (2007). Bayesian overconfidence. Heifetz, A., C. Shannon, and Y. Spiegel (2007). The dynamic evolution of preferences. Economic Theory 32(2), 251. HESA, H. E. S. A. (2017). Qualifications obtained. Johnson, D. D. and J. H. Fowler (2011). The evolution of overconfidence. Nature 477(7364), 317–320. Kruger, J. (1999). Lake wobegon be gone! the below-average effec and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. Journal of personality and social psychology 77(2), 221. Kruger, J. and D. Dunning (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of personality and social psychology 77(6), 1121. Larrick, R. P., K. A. Burson, and J. B. Soll (2007). Social comparison and confidence: When thinking you are better than average predicts overconfidence (and when it does not). Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 102(1), 76 – 94. Special Issue on Social Comparison Processes. Malmendier, U. and G. Tate (2005). Ceo overconfidence and corporate investment. The journal of finance 60(6), 2661–2700. Moore, D. A. and P. J. Healy (2008). The trouble with overconfidence. Psychological review 115(2), 502. 41 Moore, D. A. and T. G. Kim (2003). Myopic social prediction and the solo comparison effect. Journal of personality and social psychology 85(6), 1121. Moore, D. A. and D. A. Small (2007). Error and bias in comparative judgment: on being both better and worse than we think we are. Journal of personality and social psychology 92(6), 972. Murad, Z., M. Sefton, and C. Starmer (2016, Feb). How do risk attitudes affect measured confidence? Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 52(1), 21–46. Niederle, M. (2017). A gender agenda: A progress report on competitiveness. American Economic Review 107(5), 115–19. Niederle, M. and L. Vesterlund (2007). Do women shy away from competition? do men compete too much? The Quaterly Journal of Economics, 1067–1101. Odean, T. (1998). Are investors reluctant to realize their losses? The Journal of finance 53(5), 1775–1798. Pikulina, E., L. Renneboog, and P. Tobler (2016). Do confident individuals generally work harder? Pikulina, E., L. Renneboog, and P. N. Tobler (2017). Overconfidence and investment: An experimental approach. Journal of Corporate Finance 43, 175–192. Puri, M. and D. T. Robinson (2007). Optimism and economic choice. Journal of Financial Economics 86(1), 71 – 99. Rosovsky, H. and M. Hartley (2002). Evaluation and the academy: Are we doing the right thing. Schwardmann, P. and J. J. Van der Weele (2016). Deception and self-deception. Working Paper. Selten, R. (1998). Axiomatic characterization of the quadratic scoring rule. Experimental Economics 1(1), 43–62. Smith, A. (2013, Sep). Estimating the causal effect of beliefs on contributions in repeated public good games. Experimental Economics 16(3), 414–425. van Veldhuizen, R. (2017). Gender differences in tournament choices: Risk preferences, overconfidence or competitiveness? Technical report, Discussion Paper. |
URI: | https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/id/eprint/84743 |